Interference in visual perception by verbal and spatial cognitive activity

  1. Pérez Moreno, Elisa 1
  2. Conchillo Jiménez, Ángela 1
  3. Recarte Goldaracena, Miguel Ángel 1
  1. 1 Universidad Complutense de Madrid
    info

    Universidad Complutense de Madrid

    Madrid, España

    ROR 02p0gd045

Revista:
The Spanish Journal of Psychology

ISSN: 1138-7416

Año de publicación: 2011

Volumen: 14

Número: 2

Páginas: 556-568

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.5209/REV_SJOP.2011.V14.N2.4 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso abierto editor

Otras publicaciones en: The Spanish Journal of Psychology

Objetivos de desarrollo sostenible

Resumen

Mediante dos experimentos hemos puesto a prueba la hipótesis de que el procesamiento cognitivo basado en imaginería espacial produce más deterioro en la percepción visual que el procesamiento basado en códigos verbales. Hemos estudiado el efecto de dos tareas cognitivas, una de imaginería espacial y otra verbal, sobre la percepción visual. En el primer experimento, con 30 participantes, analizamos la carga mental y el comportamiento ocular en ambas tareas cognitivas. En el segundo experimento, con 29 participantes, estudiamos el efecto de ambas tareas sobre una tarea de búsqueda visual, usando el paradigma experimental de doble tarea. La tarea verbal presentó mayor carga mental que la de imaginería espacial cuando ambas tareas se realizaron junto con la tarea de búsqueda visual y hubo un mayor deterioro en la detección de estímulos con la tarea verbal. Concluimos que (1) las tareas cognitivas producen un importante deterioro en capacidades de búsqueda visual e identificación de estímulos visuales; (2) este deterioro tiene dos componentes: (a) búsqueda ineficiente, asociada con alteraciones del patrón de mirada mientras se desarrollan tareas cognitivas, y (b) interferencia general, no específica de códigos espaciales, en el proceso de identificación de estímulos mirados; (3) esta interferencia cognitiva está relacionada con la carga mental o esfuerzo requerido por tareas cognitivas.

Información de financiación

This study is part of a project funded by the Spanish Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia, reference number SEJ2007-63495.

Financiadores

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Beatty, J. (1982). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychological Bulletin, 91(2), 276-292. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.91.2.276
  • Beck, D. M., & Lavie, N. (2005). Look here but ignore what you see: Effects of distractors at fixation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 51(3), 592-607. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.3.592 (Pubitemid 41017478)
  • Cartwright-Finch, U., & Lavie, N. (2007). The role of perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Cognition, 102(3), 321-340. doi:10.1016/j. cognition.2006.01.002 (Pubitemid 46107406)
  • De Fockert, J. W., Rees, G, Frith, C. D., & Lavie, N. (2001). The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science, 291, 1803-1806. doi:10.1126/science.1056496 (Pubitemid 32198988)
  • De Waard, D. (1996). The measurement of drivers' mental workload (Doctoral thesis). Retrieved from http://dissertations.ub.rug.nl/FILES/ faculties/gmw/1996/d.de.waard/09-thesis.pdf
  • Farah, M. J. (1985). Psychophysical evidence for a shared representational medium for mental images and percepts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114(1), 91-103. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.114.1.91
  • Fougnie, D., & Marois, R. (2007). Executive working memory load induces inattentional blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(1), 142-147. doi:10.3758/BF03194041
  • Goldstein, E. B. (2002). Sensation and perception. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
  • Han, S. H., & Kim, M. S. (2004). Visual search does not remain efficient when executive working memory is working. Psychological Science, 15(9), 623-628. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00730.x (Pubitemid 39234378)
  • Hoeks, B., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1993). Pupillary dilation as a measure of attention: A quantitative system analysis. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 25, 16-26. doi:10.3758/BF03204445
  • Iqbal, S. T., Zheng, X. S., & Bailey, B. P. (2004). Taskevoked pupillary response to mental workload in human-computer interaction. CHI 04 (pp. 1477-1480). New York, NY: AMS Press. Retrieved from http://portal.acm.org/ citation.cfm?id=986094
  • Irwin, D. E., & Brockmole, J. R. (2000). Mental rotation is suppressed during saccadic eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7(4), 654-661.doi:10.3758/BF03213003
  • Janisse, M. P. (1977). Pupillometry: The psychology of the pupillary response. New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1988). Aspects of a cognitive neuroscience of mental imagery. Science, 240, 1621-1621. doi:10.1126/science. 3289115
  • Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 451-68. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.451
  • Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 75-82. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004 (Pubitemid 40164472)
  • Lavie, N. (2006). The role of perceptual load in visual awareness. Brain Research, 1080(1), 91-100. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.023
  • Lavie, N., & De Fockert, J. W. (2005). The role of working memory in attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(4), 669-674. doi:10.3758/BF03196756 (Pubitemid 43119956)
  • Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 339-354. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339 (Pubitemid 39203047)
  • Leigh, R. J., & Zee, D. S. (1999). The neurology of eye movements. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Macdonald, J. S. P., & Lavie, N. (2008). Load induced blindness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(5), 1078-1091. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1078
  • Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Most, S. B., Scholl, B. J., Clifford, E. R., & Simons, D. J. (2005). What you see is what you set: Sustained inattentional blindness and the capture of awareness. Psychological Review, 112, 217-242. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.112.1. 217 (Pubitemid 40131526)
  • Näätänen, R. (1992). Attention and brain function. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Noë, A., & O'Regan, J. K. (2000). Perception, attention, and the grand illusion. Psyche, 6(15), 6-15.
  • Oh, S. H., & Kim, M. S. (2004). The role of spatial working memory in visual search efficiency. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11(2), 275-281. doi:10.3758/BF03196570
  • Pappas, J. M., Fishel, S. R., Moss, J. D., Hicks, J. M., & Leech, T. D. (2005). An eye-tracking approach to inattentional blindness. In Proccedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (Vol. 49, pp. 1658-1662). Orlando, FL: HFES. (Pubitemid 351742560)
  • Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. New York, NY: Scientific American Library.
  • Recarte, M. A., & Nunes, L. M. (2000). Effects of verbal and spatial-imagery tasks on eye fixations while driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied, 6(1), 31-43. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.6.1.31
  • Recarte, M. A., & Nunes, L. M. (2002). Parpadeo durante la conducción: efectos de la carga mental y del tiempo conduciendo [Blink during driving: mental load and time driving effects]. Vigiliay sueño, 14, 161-167.
  • Recarte, M. A., & Nunes, L. M. (2003). Mental workload while driving: Effects on visual search, discrimination, and decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied, 9(2), 119-133. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.9.2.119 (Pubitemid 37177407)
  • Recarte, M. A., Pérez, E., Conchillo, A., & Nunes, L. M. (2008). Mental workload and visual impairment: differences between pupil, blink, and subjective rating. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 11(2), 374-385.
  • Rees, G, Frith, C. D., & Lavie, N. (1997). Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. Science, 278, 1616-1619. doi:10.1126/science.278.5343.1616 (Pubitemid 27516278)
  • Ryu, K., & Myung, R. (2005). Evaluation of mental workload with a combined measure based on physiological indices during a dual task of tracking and mental arithmetic. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 55(11), 991-1009. doi:10.1016/j.ergon.2005.04.005. (Pubitemid 41765829)
  • Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059-1074. doi:10.1068/p2952.
  • Stern, J. A., Boyer, D., & Schroeder, D. (1994). Blink rate: a possible measure of fatigue. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 36(2), 285-297. (Pubitemid 24247276)
  • Todd, J. J., Fougnie, D., & Marois, R. (2005). Visual short-term memory load suppresses temporo-parietal junction activity and induces inattentional blindness. Psychological Science, 16(12), 965-972. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01645.x (Pubitemid 43060259)
  • Von Helmholtz, H. (1924). Treatise on physiological optics. Rochester, NY: Optical Society of America.
  • Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Parasuraman & R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 63-102). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Wickens, C. D. (1992). Engineering Psychology and human performance. New York, NY: HarperCollins.
  • Woodman, G F., & Luck, S. J. (2004). Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11(2), 269-274. doi:10.3758/BF03196569. (Pubitemid 39436169)
  • Woodman, G F., Vogel, E. K., & Luck, S. J. (2001). Visual search remains efficient when visual working memory is full. Psychological Science, 12(3), 219-224. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00339. (Pubitemid 33653534)