El Desarrollo de la Marcha Infantil como Proceso de Aprendizaje

  1. Martín Casas, Patricia
  2. Meneses Monroy, Alfonso
  3. Benéit Montesinos, Juan Vicente
  4. Atín Arratibel, María de los Angeles
Zeitschrift:
Acción psicológica

ISSN: 1578-908X

Datum der Publikation: 2014

Titel der Ausgabe: Homenaje al profesor Dionisio Manga Rodríguez: contribuciones al desarrollo de la neuropsicología del siglo XX y XXI

Ausgabe: 11

Nummer: 1

Seiten: 45-54

Art: Artikel

DOI: 10.5944/AP.11.1.13866 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openOpen Access editor

Andere Publikationen in: Acción psicológica

Ziele für nachhaltige Entwicklung

Zusammenfassung

Children gait development is a particularly complex process in which motor, perceptual, and cognitive factors interact. From the moment of birth, the child integrates the information he receives and builds up a progressive cortical control on the innate neuromotor patterns to develop different forms of locomotion until achieving independent gait. These ideas are not new but they are currently being reviewed and have encouraged numerous changes in the study of child development. Considering gait acquisition as a learning process in changing situations allows to understand gait as an action which involves the acquisition of new knowledge and interaction capabilities. This paper summarizes the main body of knowledge about this learning process and intends to highlight gait alterations as indicators of development difficulties, requiring interdisciplinary assessment and therapeutic approach.

Bibliographische Referenzen

  • Adolph, K. E. (2008). Learning to move. Current Di-rections in Psychological Science, 17(3), 213-218.
  • Adolph, K. E. y Avolio, A. M. (2000). Walking in-fants adapt locomotion to changing body di-mensions. Journal of Experimental Psycholo-gy: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1148-1166.
  • Adolph, K. E., Eppler, M. A., Marin, L., Weise, I. B. y Wechsler, M. (2000). Exploration in the service of prospective control. Infant Behavior and De-velopment, 23, 441-460.
  • Adolph, K. E., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Ishak, S., Karasik, L. B. y Lobo, S. A. (2008). Locomo-tor experience and use of social information are posture specific. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1705-1714.
  • Adolph, K. E., Vereijken, B. y Shrout, P. E. (2003). What changes in infant walking and why. Child Development, 74(2), 475-497.
  • Angulo-Kinzler, R. M., Ulrich, B. y Thelen, E. (2002). Three-month-old infants can select spe-cific leg motor solutions. Motor Control, 6(1), 52-68.
  • Assaiante, C. y Amblard, B. (1992). Peripheral vi-sion and age-related differences in dynamic balance. Human Movement Science, 11, 533-548.
  • Assaiante, C. y Amblard, B. (1996). Visual factors in the child gait: effects on locomotor skills. Per-ceptual and Motor Skills, 83, 1019-1041.
  • Assaiante, C., Mallau, S., Viel, S., Jover, M. y Sch-mitz, C. (2005). Development of postural control in healthy children: a functional approach. Neu-ral Plasticity, 12(2-3), 109-118.
  • Berger, S. E. y Adolph, K. E. (2007). Learning and development in infant locomotion. Progress in Brain Research, 164, 237-255.
  • Brenière, Y. y Bril, B. (1998). Development of pos-tural control of gravity forces in children during the first 5 years of walking. Experimental Brain Research, 121, 255-262.
  • Bril, B. y Brenière, Y. (1992). Postural requirements and progression velocity in young walkers. Jour-nal of Motor Behavior, 24, 105-116.
  • Bril, B. y Ledebt, A. (1998). Head coordination as a means to assist sensory integration in learning walk. Neurosci Biobehav Rev Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 22(4), 555-563.
  • Cavagna, G. A., Franzetti, P. y Fuchimoto, T. (1983). The mechanics of walking in children. Journal of Physiology 343, 323-339.
  • Clark, J. E., Whitall, J. y Phillips, S. J. (1988). Hu-man interlimb coordination: The first 6 months of independent walking. Developmental Psycho-biology, 21, 445-456.
  • Clearfield, M. W., Osborne, C. N. y Mullen, M. (2008). Learning by looking: Infants’ social looking behavior across the transition from crawling to walking. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100, 297-307.
  • Cooke, D. W. y Thelen, E. (1987). Newborn step-ping: A review of puzzling infant co-ordination. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 29(3), 399-404.
  • Cherng, R. J., Liang, L. Y., Chen, Y. J. y Chen, J. Y. (2009). The effects of a motor and a cognitive concurrent task on walking in children with developmental coordination disorder. Gait & Posture, 29, 204-207.
  • Dal Molin, G. (1994). Il cammino: studiarlo per poterlo rieducare [Gait: study to be able to re-educate]. Riabilitazione e apprendimento, 3/4, 153-162.
  • Fogel, A. y Thelen, E. (1987). Development of early expressive and communicative action. Reinterupting the evidence from a dynamic systems perspective. Child Development, 23, 747.
  • Forssberg, H. (1985). Ontogeny of human locomo-tor control. I. Infant stepping, supported loco-motion, and transition to independent locomo-tion. Experimental Brain Research, 57, 480-493.
  • Hirschfeld, H. y Forssberg, H. (1992). Development of anticipatory postural adjustements during locomotion in children. Journal of Neurophysio-logy, 68, 542-549.
  • Ivanenko, Y. P., Dominici, N., Cappellini, G. y Lacquaniti, F. (2005). Kinematics in newly wal-king toddlers does no depend upon postural sta-bility. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 754-763.
  • Jensen, J. L., Schneider, K., Ulrich, B. D., Zernicke, R. F. y Thelen, E. (1994). Adaptative dynamics of the leg movement patterns of human infants: I. The effects of posture on spontaneous kic-king. Journal of Motor Behavior, 26(4), 303-312.
  • Joh, A. S. y Adolph, K. E. (2006). Learning from falling. Child Dev, 77(1), 89-102.
  • Kamm, K., Thelen, E. y Jensen, J. L. (1990). A dina-mical systems approach to motor development. Physical Therapy, 70, 763-775.
  • Lam, T., Wolstenholme, C. y Yang, J. F. (2003). How do infants adapt to loading of the limb during the swing phase of stepping? Journal of Neuro-physiology, 89, 1920-1928.
  • Lamb, T. y Yang, J. F. (2000). Could differents di-rections of infant stepping be controlled by the same locomotor central pattern generator? Jour-nal of Neurophysiology, 83, 2814-2824.
  • Ledebt, A. y Brenière, Y. (1994). Dynamic implica-tions of anatomical and mechanical parameters in gait iniciation process in children. Human Movement Science, 13, 801-815.
  • Ledebt, A., Bril, B. y Brenière, Y. (1998). The build-up of anticipatory behaviour. An analysis of the development of gait iniciation in children. Expe-rimental Brain Research, 120(1), 9-17.
  • Ledebt, A., Bril, B. y Wiener-Vacher, S. (1995). Trunk and head stabilization during the first months of independent walking. NeuroReport, 6, 1737-1740.
  • Leonard, E. L. (1990). Early motor development and control: Foundations for independent wal-king. In G. L. Smith (Ed.), Gait in Rehabilita-tion. Clinics in Physical Therapy (pp. 121-140). New York: Churchill Livingstone.
  • Metcalfe, J. S. y Clark, J. E. (2000). Sensory infor-mation affords exploration of posture in newly walking infants and toddlers. Infant Behavior & Development, 23, 391-405.
  • Petersen, T. H., Kliim-Due, M., Farmer, S. F. y Nielsen, J. B. (2010). Childhood development of common drive to a human leg muscle during ankle dorsiflexion and gait. Journal of Physiolo-gy, 588(Pt 22), 4387-4400.
  • Portellano, J. A. (2007). Neuropsicología infantil. Madrid: Síntesis.
  • Roncesvalles, M. N. C., Woollacott, M. H. y Jensen, J. L. (2001). Development of lower extremity kine-tics for balance control in infants and young chil-dren. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33(2), 180-192.
  • Schneider, K., Zernicke, R. F., Ulrich, B. D., Jensen, J. L. y Thelen, E. (1990). Understanding move-ment control in infants through the analysis of limb intersegmental dynamics. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22(4), 493-520.
  • Shumway-Cook, A. y Woollacott, M. H. (2007). Mo-tor Control: Translating Research Into Clinical Practice. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams y Wilkins.
  • Sundermier, L., Woollacott, M. H., Roncesvalles, N. y Jensen, J. (2001). The development of balance control in children: comparisons of EMG and kinetic variables and chronological and develop-mental groupings. Experimental Brain Research, 136, 340-350.
  • Thelen, E. (1995). Motor development. A new synthesis. American Psychologist, 50(2), 79-95.Thelen, E. y Cooke, D. W. (1987). Relationship bet-ween newborn stepping and later walking: A new interpretation. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 29(3), 380-393.
  • Thelen, E., Kelso, J. A. S. y Fogel, A. (1987). Self-organizing systems and infant motor develop-ment. Developmental Review, 7, 39-65.
  • Thelen, E., Schöner, G., Scheier, C. y Smith, L. B. (2001). The dynamics of embodyment: a field theory of infant perseverative reaching. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, 24(1), 1-34.
  • Thelen, E., Ulrich, B. D. y Niles, D. (1987). Bilateral coordination in human infants: stepping on a split-belt treadmill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 405-410.
  • Williams, C. M., Tinley, P. y Curtin, M. (2010). Idiopathic toe walking and sensory processing disfunction. Journal of FootandAnkle Research, 3, 16-20.
  • Yang, J. F., Lam, T., Pang, M. Y. C., Lamont, E., Musselman, K. y Seinen, E. (2004). Infant step-ping: a window to the behaviour of the human pattern generator for walking. Canadian Journal of Physiology andPharmacology, 82, 662-674.
  • Zelazo, P. R. (1983). The development of walking: New findings and old assumptions. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15(2), 99-137.
  • Zelazo, P. R. y Leonard, E. L. (1983). The dawn of active thought. In K. W. Fisher (Ed.), Le-vels and Transitions in Children’s Development(pp. 37-50). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
  • Zwart, R., Ledebt, A., Fong, B. F., De Vries, H. y Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2005). The affordance of gap crossing in toddlers. Infant Behavior & De-velopment, 28, 145-154