Verbal forward digit span in Spanish population

  1. Sebastián, María Victoria
  2. Mediavilla, Roberto
Revista:
Psicothema

ISSN: 0214-9915

Año de publicación: 2015

Volumen: 27

Número: 2

Páginas: 93-98

Tipo: Artículo

Otras publicaciones en: Psicothema

Resumen

Antecedentes: las personas mayores se quejan con frecuencia de las dificultades que tienen para recordar números de teléfono o para marcarlos en el orden correcto. Este estudio investiga el desarrollo de la amplitud verbal de dígitos en orden directo en adultos y personas mayores en población española, como índice de uno de los componentes del modelo de memoria operativa de Baddeley: el lazo fonológico, que precisamente ilustra estos dos aspectos. Método: la tarea verbal de amplitud de dígitos en orden directo se administró a una muestra incidental de 987 participantes, entre 35 y 90 años. Se definió como la longitud mayor de series de dígitos repetidas correctamente en, al menos, dos de las tres series. Variables demográficas, género y nivel educativo también se examinaron. Resultados: el ANOVA mostró que los tres factores principales (grupo de edad, género y nivel educativo) fueron significativos, pero ninguna de las interacciones alcanzó la significación. La amplitud verbal de dígitos en orden directo desciende durante el ciclo vital, pero tanto género como nivel educativo afectaron ligeramente. Conclusiones: el lazo fonológico está afectado por la edad. La amplitud verbal de dígitos en orden directo fue, en general, menor que la encontrada en otros estudios.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Aparecida, M., & Rocha de Vasconcellos, S. (2010). Phonological working memory: A comparative study between different age groups. Jornal da Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia, 23(3), 245-251.
  • Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. VIII (pp. 47-89). New York: Academic Press.
  • Baddeley, A.D., Eysenck, M.W., & Anderson, M.C. (2014). Memory (2nd edition). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Baddeley, A.D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14(6), 575-589.
  • Banken, J. A. (1985). Clinical utility of considering digits forward and digits backward as separate components of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41(5), 686-691.
  • Bopp, K.L., & Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal memory span: A meta-analysis. Journal of Gerontology, 60B(5), 223-233.
  • Chen, Z.Y., Cowell, P.E., Varley, R., & Wang, Y.C. (2009). A cross-language study of verbal and visuospatial working memory span. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(4), 385-391.
  • Choi, H.J., Lee, D.Y., Seo, E.H., Jo, M.K., Sohn, B.K., Choe, Y.M., et al. (2014). A normative study of the digit span in an educationally diverse elderly population. Psychiatry Investigation, 11, 39-43.
  • Della Sala, S., Cocchini, G., Logie, R.H., Allerhand, M., & MacPherson, S.E. (2010). Dual task during encoding, maintenance, and retrieval in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 19(2), 503-515.
  • Fisk, J.E., & Warr, P. (1996). Working memory: The role of perceptual speed, central executive and phonological loop. Psychology and Aging, 11(2), 316-323.
  • Gathercole, S.E., & Alloway, T.P. (2008). Working memory and learning. A practical guide for teachers. London: Sage Publications.
  • Gregoire, J., & Van der Linden, M. (1997). Effect of age on forward and backward digit spans. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition: A Journal on Normal and Dysfunctional Development, 4(2), 140-149.
  • Hale, S., Rose, N.S., Myerson, J., Strube, N.J., Sommers, M., Tye-Murray, N. et al. (2011). The structure of working memory abilities across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 92-110.
  • Hickman, S.E., Howieson, D.B., Dame, A., Sexton, G., & Kaye, J. (2000). Longitudinal analysis of the effects of the aging process on neuropsychological test Performance in the Healthy Young-Old and Oldest-Old. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17(3), 323-337.
  • Kaufman, A.S., McLean, J.E., & Reynolds, C.R. (1988). Sex, race, residence, region and education differences on the 11 WAIS-R subtests. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(2), 231-247.
  • Kumar, N., & Priyadarshi, B. (2013). Differential effect of aging on verbal and visuo-spatial working memory. Aging and Disease, 4(4), 170-178.
  • Monaco, M., Costa, A., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2013). Forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: Standardization and normative data from an Italian adult population. Neurological Sciences, 34(5), 749-754.
  • Myerson, J., Emery, L., White, D.A., & Hale, S. (2003). Effects of age, domain, and processing demands on memory span: Evidence for differential decline. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 10(1), 20-27.
  • Olazarán, J., Jacobs, D.M., & Stern, Y. (1996). Comparative study of visual and verbal short-term memory in English and Spanish speakers: Testing a linguistic hypothesis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 105-110.
  • Otrosky-Solís, F., & Lozano, A. (2006). Digit Span: Effect of education and culture. International Journal of Psychology, 41(5), 333-341.
  • Peña-Casanova, J., Quiñones-Úbeda, S., Quintana-Aparicio, M., Aguilar, M., Badenes, D., Molinuevo, J.L., et al. (2009). Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): Norms for verbal span, visuospatial span, letter and number sequencing, trail making test, and symbol digit modalities test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(4), 307-319.
  • Philips, L.H., & Hamilton, C. (2001). The working memory model in adult aging research. In J. Andrade (Ed.), Working Memory in Perspective. Hove: Psychology Press.
  • Ryan, J.J., López, S.J., & Paolo, A.M. (1996). Digit span performance of persons 75-96 years of age: Base rates and associations with selected demographic variables. Psychological Assessment, 8(3), 324-327.
  • Salthouse, T.A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103(3), 403-428.
  • Sebastián, M.V., & Hernández-Gil, L. (2010). A comparison of memory and executive functions in Alzheimer disease and the frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Psicothema, 22(3), 424-429.
  • Sebastián, M.V., & Hernández-Gil, L. (2012). Developmental pattern of digit span in Spanish population. Psicothema, 24(2), 183-187.
  • Stuart-Hamilton, I. (2012). The Psychology of Ageing: An introduction (5th edition). London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
  • Wechsler, D. (1955). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS). New York: The Psychological Corporation [Spanish Version, Madrid: TEA Ediciones, 1998 (13th edition)].
  • Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised (WAIS-R). San Antonio, Texas: The Psychological Corporation.
  • Wechsler, D. (1997a). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Third Edition (WAIS-III). San Antonio, Texas: The Psychological Corporation [Spanish Version, Madrid: TEA Ediciones, 1999].
  • Wechsler, D. (1997b). Wechsler Memory Scale - Third Edition (WMS-III). San Antonio, Texas: The Psychological Corporation [Spanish Version, Madrid: TEA Ediciones, 2004
  • Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV). San Antonio, Texas: Pearson, Inc. [Spanish Version, Madrid: NCS Pearson, Inc., 2012.
  • Wechsler, D. (2009). Wechsler Memory Scale - Fourth Edition (WMS-IV). San Antonio, Texas: Pearson, Inc. [Spanish Version, Madrid: NCS Pearson, Inc., 2013].
  • Wilde, N. J., Strauss, E., & Tuslky, D.S. (2004). Memory span on the Wechsler Scales. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26(4), 539-549.