A digital generation in scrutiny. Children’s ideas about the internet and the online information

  1. Ileana Enesco 1
  2. Carla Sebastián-Enesco 1
  3. Silvia Guerrero 2
  4. Elena Varea 1
  5. Paula Barrios 1
  1. 1 Universidad Complutense de Madrid
    info

    Universidad Complutense de Madrid

    Madrid, España

    ROR 02p0gd045

  2. 2 Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha
    info

    Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha

    Ciudad Real, España

    ROR https://ror.org/05r78ng12

Revista:
Journal for the Study of Education and Development, Infancia y Aprendizaje

ISSN: 0210-3702 1578-4126

Año de publicación: 2022

Volumen: 45

Número: 4

Páginas: 842-869

Tipo: Artículo

Otras publicaciones en: Journal for the Study of Education and Development, Infancia y Aprendizaje

Resumen

Internet se ha convertido en un entorno clave de aprendizaje y de ocio en niños y niñas de edades cada vez más tempranas. Poco se sabe, sin embargo, sobre la comprensión que tienen sobre este entorno virtual durante la niñez. En este trabajo, realizamos entrevistas semi-estructuradas a 111 niños y niñas de cinco a nueve años con el objetivo de entender sus nociones sobre internet, sobre la autoría del contenido online y la fiabilidad que atribuyen al mismo. Teniendo en cuenta los avances sociocognitivos durante este período evolutivo, esperábamos una mejora significativa en la noción de internet. Sin embargo, los resultados muestran diferencias de edad modestas y solo respecto a algunos aspectos básicos (ej., las funciones de internet). Encontramos, por el contrario, que las creencias erróneas sobre lo que es internet y las ideas ingenuas sobre la fiabilidad de los contenidos online estaban presentes en todas las edades. Solo una minoría de participantes de mayor edad contemplaba el riesgo de encontrar información no fiable en internet y alertaban de la necesidad de consultar otras fuentes, en caso de duda. Se discute la necesidad de abordar las ideas erróneas de los niños al menos desde el inicio de la educación primaria, período en el cual la mayoría de ellos ya son capaces de construir muchos de los conceptos abordados en este estudio dentro un contexto de instrucción formal.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Anderson, E. L., Steen, E., & Stavropoulos, V. (2017). Internet use and problematic internet use: A systematic review of longitudinal research trends in adolescence and emergent adulthood. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 22(4), 430–454. https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2016.1227716 [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. MIT press. [Google Scholar]
  • Cushman, F., Sheketoff, R., Wharton, S., & Carey, S. (2013). The development of intent-based moral judgment. Cognition, 127(1), 6–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.008 [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • D’Esterre, A. P., Rizzo, M. T., & Killen, M. (2019). Unintentional and intentional falsehoods: The role of morally relevant theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 177, 53–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.07.013 [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Daoud, R., Starkey, L., Eppel, E., Vo, T. D., & Sylvester, A. (2020). The educational value of internet use in the home for school children: A systematic review of literature. Journal of Research on Technology in Education, 53(4), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/15391523.2020.1783402 [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Derevensky, J. L., Hayman, V., & Gilbeau, L. (2019). Behavioral addictions: Excessive gambling, gaming, internet, and smartphone use among children and adolescents. Pediatric Clinics, 66(6), 1163–1182. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-1758H [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • DiSessa, A. (2014). A history of conceptual change research: Threads and fault lines. In R. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 88–108). Cambridge University Press. [Crossref], [Google Scholar]
  • Dodge, A. M., Husain, N., & Duke, N. K. (2011). Connected kids? K—2 children’s use and understanding of the internet. Language Arts, 89(2), 86–98. https://www.jstor.org/stable/41804322 [Google Scholar]
  • Edwards, S., Nolan, A., Henderson, M., Mantilla, A., Plowman, L., & Skouteris, H. (2018). Young children’s everyday concepts of the internet: A platform for cyber‐safety education in the early years. British Journal of Educational Technology, 49(1), 45–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjet.12529 [Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Eskelä-Haapanen, S., & Kiili, C. (2019). ‘It goes around the world’ – Children’s understanding of the internet. Nordic Journal of Digital Literacy, 14(3–4), 175–187. https://doi.org/10.18261/.1891-943x-2019-03-04-07 [Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Ey, L. A., & Glenn Cupit, C. (2011). Exploring young children’s understanding of risks associated with internet usage and their concepts of management strategies. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 9(1), 53–65. https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718X10367471 [Crossref], [Google Scholar]
  • Gentile, A., Bailey, K., Bavelier, D., Brockmyer, J. F., Cash, H., Coyne, S. M., Doan, A., Grant, D. S., Green, C. S., Griffiths, M., Markle, T., Petry, N. M., Prot, S., Rae, C. D., Rehbein, F., Rich, M., Sullivan, D., Woolley, E., & Young, K. (2017). internet gaming disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 140(Suppl. 2), S81–S85. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-1758H [Crossref], [PubMed], [Google Scholar]
  • German, T., & Johnson, S. (2002). Function and the origin of the design stance. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3(3), 279–300. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0303_2 [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Goswami, U. (2019). Cognitive development and cognitive neuroscience: The learning brain. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315684734 [Crossref], [Google Scholar]
  • Guerrero, S., Sebastián-Enesco, C., Morales, I., Varea, E., & Enesco, I. (2020). (In) sensitivity to accuracy? Children’s and adults’ decisions about who to trust: The teacher or the internet. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.551131 [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Holloway, D., Green, L., & Livingstone, S. (2013). Zero to eight. Young children and their internet use. LSE, EU Kids. https://ro.ecu.edu.au/ecuworks2013/929 [Google Scholar]
  • Hughes, C., & Leekam, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reflections and new directions for studies of typical and atypical development. Social Development, 13(4), 590–619. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00285.x [Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2002). Young children’s naive thinking about the biological world. Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203759844 [Google Scholar]
  • Keil, F., Grief, M., & Kerner, R. (2007). A world apart: How concepts of the constructed world are different in representation and development. In E. Margolis, & S. Lawrence (Eds.), Creations of the Mind. Theories of artifacts and their representation (pp. 231–247). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Király, O., Potenza, O. M., Stein, D. J., King, D. L., Hodgins, D. C., Saunders, J. B., Griffiths, M. D., Gjoneska, B., Billieux, J., Brand, M., Abbott, M. W., Chamberlain, S. R., Corazza, O., Burkauskas, J., Sales, C. M. D., Montag, C., Lochner, C., Grünblatt, E., Wegmann, E., … Demetrovics, Z. (2020). Preventing problematic internet use during the COVID-19 pandemic: Consensus guidance. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 100, 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2020.152180 [Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Krippendorff, K. (2018). Content analysis. An introduction to its methodology (4th ed.). Sage Pub. [Google Scholar]
  • Leiner, B. M., Cerf, V. G., Clark, D. D., Kahn, R. E., Kleinrock, L., Lynch, D. C., Postel, J., Roberts, L. G., & Wolff, S. (2009). A brief history of the internet. ACM SIGCOMM Computer Communication Review, 39(5), 22–31. https://doi.org/10.1145/1629607.1629613 [Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Limón, M., & Mason, L. (Eds.). (2002). Reconsidering conceptual change: Issues in theory and practice. Kluwer Academic Publishers. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-306-47637-1 [Crossref], [Google Scholar]
  • Livingstone, S., Ólafsson, K., O’Neill, B., & Donoso, V. (2012) Towards a better internet for children: Findings and recommendations from EU Kids Online to inform the CEO coalition. LSE, EU Kids. http://eprints.lse.ac.uk/44213/ [Google Scholar]
  • Lovato, S. B., & Piper, A. M. (2019). Young children and voice search: What we know from human-computer interaction research. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(8), 1–5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00008 [PubMed], [Google Scholar]
  • Mertala, P. (2019). Young children’s conceptions of computers, code, and the internet. International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction, 19, 56–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijcci.2018.11.003 [Crossref], [Google Scholar]
  • Murray, T., & Buchanan, R. (2018). ‘The internet is all around us’: How children come to understand the internet. Digital Culture & Education, 10(1), 1–21. https://bit.ly/2RP6mqX [Google Scholar]
  • Pengnate, S., & Antonenko, P. (2013). A multimethod evaluation of online trust and its interaction with metacognitive awareness: An emotional design perspective. International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, 29(9), 582–593. https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2012.735185 [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1967). La psychologie de l’enfant. P.U.F. (The psychology of the child. Basic books. 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • Pozo, J. I. (1987). La historia se repite: Las concepciones espontáneas sobre el movimiento y la gravedad. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 10(38), 69–87. https://doi.org/10.1080/02103702.1987.10822163 [Taylor & Francis Online], [Google Scholar]
  • Rabagliati, H., Marcus, G. F., & Pylkkänen, L. (2010). Shifting senses in lexical semantic development. Cognition, 117(1), 17–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.06.007 [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Robertson, J., Manches, A., & Pain, H. (2017). “It’s like a giant brain with a keyboard”: Children’s understandings about how computers work. Childhood Education, 93(4), 338–345. https://doi.org/10.1080/00094056.2017.1343589 [Taylor & Francis Online], [Google Scholar]
  • Scharrer, L., Bromme, R., Britt, M. A., & Stadtler, M. (2012). The seduction of easiness: How science depictions influence laypeople’s reliance on their own evaluation of scientific information. Learning and Instruction, 22(3), 231–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.11.004 [Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Vosniadou, S. (2019). The development of students’ understanding of science. Frontiers in Education, 4(32), 1–6. https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2019.00032 [Google Scholar]
  • Yan, Z. (2006). What influences children’s and adolescents’ understanding of the complexity of the internet? Developmental Psychology, 42(3), 418–428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.3.418 [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Yan, Z. (2009). Limited knowledge and limited resources: Children’s and adolescents’ understanding of the internet. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 103–115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.10.012 [Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]